Losing a wallet stuffed with cash might feel like the worst-case scenario, but researchers say you might have a better chance of getting it back than if it had no cash.
People in 38 countries, including Canada, were more likely to be honest and return a stranger鈥檚 wallet if it had money in it than if it didn't, a new study finds.
Whether there was money in them or not, people in Switzerland and Norway were the most likely to be honest and report a lost wallet; while people in Morocco and China were the least likely to report it.
- See more stories showcasing the Canadian spirit in our True North Tales newsletter
The new findings, published in the journal Science, showed that, contrary to what academic economists might think, wallets holding money were returned more often.
The authors said they actually saw people act more honest, despite incentivizing people to cheat by putting cash inside a wallet. The co-lead author of the study, University of Michigan assistant professor Alain Cohn, told CTVNews.ca, 鈥渨e really didn鈥檛 expect this.鈥
鈥淲hat was even more amazing was that this pattern seems to be a global phenomenon because it didn鈥檛 just happen in one or two countries but basically every country,鈥 he said.
To conduct the study, experimenters took 17,000 鈥渓ost鈥 wallets containing contact information for their fictional owners and handed them in to people working at public and private institutions in 355 cities in 40 countries.
In each country, researchers chose five to eight cities in various regions. 鈥淚f they all happened to be in the east coast, then we would make sure to include some on the west coast,鈥 Cohn said.
Researchers then gave transparent wallets with either no money or the relative equivalent of US$13.45 to places such as banks, post offices, hotels, police stations or museums. The team then tracked how often people used the contact information to reach out to the wallets鈥 supposed owners.
Authors wrote in their report that they were shocked that people in 38 out of 40 different countries 鈥渙verwhelmingly were more likely to report lost wallets with money than without.鈥
They also found the wallet return rates were higher when they included a key, which would mean something to the owner but not the finder.
In a press release, co-lead author Michel André Maréchal explained that people may have returned the wallets with money because of their concern for self-image.
During the trials, Mexico and Peru were the only outliers in this regard. Participants there returned wallets more often if there was no money.
'FRIENDLY CANADIANS'
In Canada, researchers performed the tests in Calgary, Edmonton, Montreal, Ottawa, Quebec City, Toronto and Vancouver.
Although Canadians only ranked in the second quartile for honesty, Cohn jokingly called us 鈥渇riendly Canadians.鈥 This was because in one case, 鈥渙ne hotel in Toronto gave our experimenter a free ticket for a museum, as a thank you when he turned in the wallet.鈥
Researchers suggested the documentation of civic honesty across the world indicates peoples鈥 concern for others outweighed their selfishness, though other factors may be at play.
"The psychological forces -- an aversion to not viewing oneself as a thief -- can be stronger than the financial ones," Maréchal said.
Researchers also conducted additional testing in cities within the United States, United Kingdom and Poland, in which they included wallets with a country鈥檚 equivalent of US $94.15. When more money was involved, they saw higher rates of honesty.
Worldwide, 51 per cent of those who were handed a wallet with the smaller amount of money reported it. But when the wallet had a larger sum of money, the rate of return increased to 72 per cent.
Researchers also surveyed a group of academic economists and asked them to predict the outcome of the study. 鈥淏ut they all got it wrong. So people seem to have a wrong model of honest behaviour," Cohn said.
He said this mattered because whenever policymakers or organizations are designing programs which require participants to be honest, they may have a 鈥渢oo pessimistic a view鈥 of recipients and needlessly place too much emphasis on financial incentives rather than psychological ones.